Discourse particles (henceforth DIPs) are short, usually unstressed, possibly phonologically reduced items, sometimes clitics, which occur in oral rather than in written speech, have little or no propositional meaning and display textual and interpersonal pragmatic functions, such as connecting current with prior talk, claiming the hearer’s attention, organizing discourse—e.g. indicating new topic, initiating or closing discourse, denoting old and new information, initiating repair—, and indexing the speaker’s stance, attitudes and evaluation towards the addressee and his/her contribution (e.g. Schiffrin 1987, Zimmermann 2011); e.g. Estonian nagu (1a), küll (1b), ikka, ju, -gi/ki (Hennoste 2000, Kärk 2014), German aber, auch, denn, doch, ja, wohl (König 1990, Zimmermann 2011), Russian že, vedʼ, =to, dak (Kiseleva and Paiar 2008, Post 2005, Zalizniak and Paducheva 2018).
1. a. Estonian DIP nagu: “partial epistemic support” (Boye 2012)
Ma olen teid nagu kusagil näinud.
1SG be.PRS1SG 2PL.PTV like somewhere see.PTCP
‘I kinda feel like I’ve seen you somewhere (= it seems, but I am not entirely sure)’ (EKG 1993: §627)
b. Estonian DIP küll: signalling a following contrast
Talv on küll läbi, kuid väljas ikka veel külm.
Winter be.PRS DIP almost over but outside.INE anyway still cold
‘Winter is almost over but it is still cold outside’ (EKSS)
Particles in general are uninflected, short unstressed words, often enclitic, some occur preferably in oral speech with little or no propositional meaning, others are essential parts of the grammar. They have many functions. In her book on functional words in Finno-Ugric, Majtinskaja (1982: 123–135) classified several groups of particles: demonstrative particles (e.g. equivalents of Russ. vot! ‘here!’), restrictive (or focus) particles (e.g. ‘only’), specifying particles (e.g. Hung. éppen ‘just, exactly’), enforcing particles (e.g. Russ. že, Finn. =pA, =hAn), degree particles (‘almost, hardly’), affirmative particles (e.g. Est. kull ‘of course, well’), negation particles (‘not’), question particles (e.g. Finn. =kO), modal particles of doubt, uncertainty, conviction, wish, admission, hortativity, subjective quotes (see also the classes of modal particles in Majtinskaja 1983: 132). Clitic particles are found as question markers (Fi. =kO, Udm. =a), as quotative markers (Ko. pö), contrastive markers (Ko. =tö, =sö), conditional sentence markers (Khant. ki, Ko. kö). In grammars they may be described as essential parts of mood forms (Kamas =zä in the conjunctive, Russian by in the conditional), they may count as equivalents (Ko. med optative particle), or they are primarily adverbs which come with a secondary function, more pragmatic than grammatical (e.g. German modal particles). DIPs expressing epistemic stance e.g. in Komi are, among others, taj ‘apparently’, ödvakö ‘hardly’, öd ‘indeed (affirmative)’, nö ‘actually, also’, žö id., and others (Bubrix 1949: 194–196); and in Udmurt dyr ‘assumedly’, ojdo ‘still’, leśa ‘well’, kaď ‘apparently’ and others (Winkler 2011: 136–137).
DIPs are a highly understudied field in most minor Uralic languages, so e.g. for Votic and Ingrian the general study by Majtinskaja (1982) lists some particles, but does not offer any analysis. The two most known Votic and Ingrian descriptions by Ariste (1968) and Laanest (1978) do not even mention the particles. A preliminary description of Votic discourse particles is provided by Rozhanskiy (2005) and by Markus and Rozhanskiy (2017: 623-627); the research is based on the corpus of texts collected in the 2000s. Thus, there are cross-linguistic descriptive studies like Majtinskaja (1982, 1983), very few single studies like Erina 1997 for Mordvin, but these studies quite superficially describe specific functions and distribution of the markers. The current project proposal tackles at the collection and classification of discourse particles in order to improve their description in grammars, especially their “co-operation” with other grammatical categories, like moods, the understanding of their syntactic and semantic scope and their position in utterances.
The background of the project are three so far independent research activities: (i) nuclear pitch accent and focus (new information) marking in Komi and Udmurt (HRP Tooni aktsent 2017–2018, G. Klumpp); (ii) New quotative indices in Permic languages, epistemic functions of quotative indexes, as part of the dissertation of Denys Teptiuk; (iii) Discourse particles in Beserman Udmurt (Iuliia Zubova, Timofey Arkhangelskiy). During these research activities it often turned out that the correct interpretation of (recorded) utterances depends on a correct understanding of epistemic DIPs as e.g. in (2a–b), but the range of these particles and their meanings and functions has not been sufficiently described.
2. a. Komi
aaa abu kolökö tajö byt’t’ö kutšöm=kö loktis
INTJ NEG maybe/apparently DEM like/as.if which=INDEF come.PST3S
‘Aaa, no maybe he’s like something (.) came from somewhere’ (gymn5, 03:27–03:31).
b. Beserman Udmurt
a soizlə pe tri čʼetyresta=a mar=a tərono=uk.
and 3PL.DAT QUOT three four.hundred[Rus]=Q what=Q pay.DEB=DIP
Ma so duno evəl=n’i, ben=a, tri čʼetyresta?
DIP that expensive.ATTR be.NEG.PRS=DIP yes=Q three four.hundred [Rus]
‘And they are supposedly to pay him three [thousand] four hundred or something like this.
Isnʼt it too expensive, right, three [thousand] four hundred?’
Particles are also essential morphemes in the expression of modal and epistemic meanings in the grammar of many languages. The more conventionalized they are, the more likely is their appearance in the grammars under categories like “verbal mood”, or “modal operators”. E.g. the particle med in Udmurt and Komi is acknowledged by grammars as a marker of optative (or jussive) whereas Komi es’kö is not acknowledged as a marker of a conditional mood. But they are often treated as extragrammatical items. The meaning of so called modal particles is often difficult to grasp, but where it regularly occurs as e.g. the Russian conditional particle by (with past tense) it is assigned a status in grammar. In Finno-Ugric languages in contact with Russian (but not only there) synthetic moods are often competing with mood particles, or they occur in combinations. In grammars one may read that a certain synthetic mood is encountered in older texts, but rare, or even extinct today, replaced by a particle, e.g. in Hill Mari (Bradley et al. in prep.). The grammar of literary languages does not cover dialectal and substandard variation. The current project wants to approach the study of particles in the field of modality epistemicity by looking at their collocations with moods, their relation to moods, paying thereby attention to language internal variation.